• A
  • A
  • A
  • АБB
  • АБB
  • АБB
  • А
  • А
  • А
  • А
  • А
Обычная версия сайта
Контакты

Адрес: 105066, г. Москва,
Старая Басманная ул., д. 21/4

 

🧭 Как до нас добраться

 

Телефон: +7 (495) 772-95-90 доб. 22734

E-mail: ling@hse.ru

Руководство
Заместитель руководителя Ахапкина Яна Эмильевна

Редакторы сайта — Наталья Борисовна Пименова, Татьяна Борисовна Казакова, Максим Олегович Бажуков, Юлия Геннадьевна Бадрызлова

Книга
Proceedings of the 3rd Workshop on NLP Applications to Field Linguistics (Field Matters 2024)

Bangkok: Association for Computational Linguistics, 2024.

Статья
Толк и толки: диахронический портрет имени с семантикой ментальной сферы

Рыжаченков И. И.

Труды института русского языка им. В.В. Виноградова. 2024. № 4 (42). С. 102-118.

Глава в книге
Python для гуманитариев, или почему программированию невозможно научиться с первой попытки

Сенина А. В.

В кн.: Будь в курсе цифровых гуманитарных исследований. Красноярск: Сибирский федеральный университет, 2024. С. 164-181.

Препринт
Exploring the Effectiveness of Methods for Persona Extraction
В печати

Konstantin Zaitsev.

arxiv.org. Computer Science. Cornell University, 2024

7 октября в Школе лингвистики состоялась открытая лекция Ирины Секериной (College of Staten Island, CUNY и НИУ ВШЭ)

Тема лекции: "What Eye Movements Reveal About Quantifier-Spreading". Язык лекции - английский.

As early as in the 1960s, Jean Piaget showed that children up to the age of 9-10 make errors interpreting sentences with the universal quantifier "every" when they are paired with pictures of objects in many-to-one correspondence. They reject sentences like "Every rabbit is in a basket" as a description of a scene with three rabbits, each in a basket, along with two extra baskets. Since then extensive language acquisition studies confirmed this finding that received a name of quantifier-spreading (q-spreading) across many languages.

We adapted the traditional sentence-picture verification task to be performed in conjunction with eye-movement recordings using the Visual World Paradigm and examined visual attention associated with q-spreading. English- (N =40; age range 8-12) and Russian-speaking (N =31, age range 5;1-6;11) children performed the task, with every modifying either the figure or ground (every rabbit vs. every basket) of locative scenes. When extra objects/empty containers were present, children performed at chance (53.8-65% correct). Errors involved more fixations to the extra objects/empty containers, time-locked to regions following the quantified noun phrase. Correct responses showed longer RTs, indicating additional processing required for quantifier restriction. Accuracy was uncorrelated with verbal or nonverbal intelligence and weakly associated with age.

Moreover, an identical eye-tracking experiment with bilingual heritage Russian-English adults (N=28, mean age 20) also revealed q-spreading errors (19%). Similar error and eye movement patterns of the two groups implicate a common cognitive source, i.e., the susceptibility of all language learners regardless of the language and age to make errors driven by a default expectation for two sets of objects to be one-to-one correspondence and inattention to sentence structure. The Visual World Paradigm made it possible to reveal that allocation of visual attention is critical in establishing can be the cause of q-spreading errors cross-linguistically.